![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
Ohad ben-ShaharOriginal visual demos that make a point |
|||||
Social conformity of a line : First coined by Kanizsa (1979), social conformity of a line refers to the tendency of locally parallel lines to group together into inseperatable wholes (indeed, social groups) in which each line looses its individual identity. Strangly, the more appealing examples that Kanizsa used to illustrate social conformity of a line were proposed much eariler (Galli and Zama 1931) while his own original demo (the vanishing church) was rather odd. Here are few novel examples of the same phenomenon...
|
|||||
|
|||||
|
|||||
|
|||||
One features of this figure is that it pushes segmentation performance to its ceiling. A much more intriguing observation is made by lowering the contrast ratio. Now, while segmentation is much more difficult (as expected from existing models), it is also evident that the saliency of the orientation edges varies significantly, despite the fact that the orientation contrast is still constant across the square's edges. This suggests that the two orientation contrasts are not the only determinant factor in orientaion-based segmentation. An much more radical demonstration is made by changing the direction of the orientaiton gradient within the figure and the ground. Now, despite the fact the orientation contrast is still constant along the square's edges (all 4 of them), two of them virtually disappear. Here is a complimentary example with the top and bottom edges virtually vanishing. Finally, here is another example of a Nothdurft square with constant orientation gradient both within regions and between them, where the saliency of the perceptual edges is anything but constant. Note in particular how the top edge is much more salient than the bottom one.
|
|||||
|
Last updated: August 19, 2002 |